Introduction
Psychology tries to understand how we think, feel, and behave. It studies the mind and actions of individuals. As the youngest science, it’s still developing and lacks the firm structure seen in older fields. Because of this, defining psychology remains controversial. Psychologists often disagree, especially during early stages of a science. These debates aren’t unusual—in fact, they help the discipline grow. Psychology once emerged from philosophy, and sometimes it still overlaps with physiology. So, without a universal definition, scholars have described psychology in different ways. Some call it the science of soul, others say it’s the science of mind, consciousness, or behavior. Let’s explore each idea and decide which one makes the most sense.
Long ago, psychology was known as “the science of soul.” This sounds poetic, but it’s scientifically weak. The term “soul” has shifted meanings over time. Aristotle once used it to describe life itself, while today, it’s tied to religious belief. It points to the immortal part of us and our relation with God. That’s why scientific thinkers reject it—it’s too rooted in faith. Psychologists aren’t against religion, but they want to keep their field free from spiritual assumptions. Science relies on observable facts, and the soul cannot be tested or measured. So, this old definition no longer works in modern psychology.
Another view says psychology is the study of mind. At first glance, that seems better. Still, “mind” can be just as vague as “soul.” People often picture it as something mystical inside the head. That makes it hard to define. However, thinkers like Hume and Titchener gave it clarity. They treated mind as a collection of mental processes—things we experience like thoughts, feelings, and decisions. For example, if a wasp stings me, I feel pain, decide to chase it, and try to kill it. These are mental processes. They’re not mysterious. They happen in real time and can be studied. This definition is more useful, but it’s incomplete. After all, we can observe our own mental processes, but not those of others. So, to understand others, we must observe their behavior.
Some define psychology as the study of consciousness. That refers to what we’re aware of—like reading a book or feeling bored. Consciousness is useful, but limited. It covers just the surface of the mind. Most of what goes on lies below awareness. Also, we can’t see the consciousness of others. This means behavior must be studied too. So, defining psychology as only consciousness is far too narrow. It leaves out unconscious processes and the actions we use to express our thoughts.
We’ll discuss following topics
1- Definition of Psychology
2- Psychology as a Science of Soul
3- Psychology as the Science of Mind
4- Psychology as a Science of Behavior
5- Dr. Watson School of psychology
Definition of Psychology
Psychology explores how people think, feel, and act. It studies the mind and human behavior in daily life. As the newest among sciences, psychology is still evolving. Because of this, experts haven’t agreed on a single definition.
From its early days, psychology has sparked intense debates. Scholars often disagree on what it truly means. These controversies, while challenging, push the field forward. Without them, psychology wouldn’t grow the way it does today.
At first, psychology was closely tied to philosophy. Over time, it started gaining its own identity. However, the line between psychology and physiology still blurs at times. Due to this confusion, defining psychology becomes even more difficult.
Many thinkers have proposed different ways to define it. Some say it’s the science of the soul. Others call it the study of the mind. A few believe it deals with consciousness. And many now see it as the science of behavior.
Each of these definitions brings a unique perspective. The idea of psychology as a science of the soul feels outdated. The term “soul” connects more to religion than science. Therefore, it doesn’t suit modern research goals.
On the other hand, calling it the science of mind helps us understand thoughts and emotions. But even this view feels limited, since mind itself can be hard to define. Consciousness, too, offers insight, though it represents only a small part of mental activity.
1. Psychology as a Science of Soul
People once believed psychology meant “the science of the soul.” This idea comes from old dictionary definitions, but it doesn’t fit today’s world. Although it’s the oldest description, age alone doesn’t make something right. The word “soul” carries heavy religious meanings that science tries to avoid.
Back in Aristotle’s time, “soul” meant the principle of life in all living beings. Today, it mostly refers to something spiritual and eternal, linked to God. That makes it more a religious term than a scientific one. Psychologists aren’t against religion, but they prefer a method based on facts, not faith.
Using “soul” brings up ideas of immortality and divine connections. These beliefs can be powerful, but science needs things it can see and test. A scientist studies what can be measured, not what people believe in their hearts. That’s why “soul” is not welcome in psychological research.
When we define psychology, we must follow scientific procedures. We need to observe, collect data, and experiment. Unfortunately, the soul can’t be placed under a microscope or observed in action. It’s not something we can test, so it doesn’t fit the goals of psychology.
For that reason, defining psychology as the science of the soul doesn’t hold up. It may have worked centuries ago, but today, researchers want clarity and proof. Psychology must stay away from religious terms to grow as a reliable discipline. As science continues to evolve, it demands definitions that are precise, neutral, and supported by observation.
So, while “soul” may remain meaningful in religious or personal discussions, it no longer serves the field of psychology. It’s simply outdated and unscientific.
2. Psychology as the Science of Mind
Some believe psychology should be called the science of mind. On the surface, it sounds more scientific than “soul,” but that’s not always the case. For many people, the word “mind” still feels vague and mysterious. It brings up images of something hidden inside the head that thinks and feels. So, this definition also faces problems.
If we ask someone on the street what “mind” means, they might say it’s the thing that thinks, decides, and worries. That sounds simple, but it doesn’t help researchers much. Psychology wants clear and testable ideas—not vague ones. Still, when scholars like Hume and Titchener talk about mind, they give it meaning. They describe it through mental processes like knowing, feeling, and willing.
Mental processes are the things that happen inside us. They aren’t hidden magic—they’re real experiences. Let’s say a wasp stings your finger. You feel pain, understand what happened, and decide to react. That’s the mind in action: thinking, feeling, and choosing. These mental processes are visible to the person experiencing them. And because they can be observed, they seem more scientific.
This idea of mind is called the empirical view. It means we can study what we experience directly. Psychologists observe patterns, run experiments, and gather data from real mental activities. That gives this definition more weight than the old idea of soul.
However, there’s still one issue. We can only study our own mental processes. To know what others feel or think, we must watch how they behave. When someone cries, we assume they’re sad—not because we feel it, but because we see it. This means behavior gives us clues to mental activity.
3. Psychology as the Science of Consciousness
Some people define psychology as the science of consciousness. That idea brings up the question—what is consciousness really? If you pause and think, you’ll realize you’re sitting at a desk, reading something, and maybe feeling curious or distracted. That awareness in the moment is what we call consciousness.
This concept helps explain part of the mind. But does it show the whole picture? Honestly, no. Consciousness only touches the surface. Our mind goes much deeper, and much of it lies outside what we’re aware of.
While conscious thoughts are easy to notice, there are plenty that stay hidden. Subconscious and unconscious processes shape how we act, even when we don’t recognize them. That’s why relying only on consciousness doesn’t offer enough. It cuts off large parts of mental life.
Also, there’s another problem. We can reflect on our own thoughts, but we can’t directly know someone else’s. We don’t get to feel their emotions or hear their silent thinking. So, to understand others, we watch what they do. Their behavior gives us clues.
If someone smiles, we guess they’re happy. If they sigh, we assume they’re tired. We don’t access their minds—but behavior shows us patterns. That’s why defining psychology through consciousness is incomplete. It skips what happens beneath awareness and ignores how we understand people through action.
4. Psychology as a Science of Behavior
Many now define psychology as the science of behavior. This view feels simple, but it carries deep meaning. Behavior includes everything a living creature does—walking, speaking, reacting, or even staying silent. These actions reflect how we adjust to the world around us.
In psychology, behavior means response to stimuli. A stimulus is anything from the outside that causes a change—like sound, light, or a sudden event. When something happens, the body and mind respond. That response becomes behavior, and psychologists study those patterns to learn more.
Unlike mental processes, behavior is observable. We may not see someone’s thoughts, but we can see how they act. When a child cries, a teacher assumes sadness. When a student refuses to speak, researchers may consider fear or anxiety. Behavior shows us clues that thoughts alone can’t offer.
That’s why behavior became central to psychology. It helps us understand others, even when they don’t speak their minds. It lets scientists conduct experiments, track changes, and test ideas. Because behavior can be measured, it fits well within science.
Still, behavior doesn’t replace everything else. Emotions and decisions happen inside us, beyond what’s visible. So, many psychologists combine behavior with mental studies. They look for balance—what we do and how we feel.
By focusing on behavior, psychology became more practical. Researchers found ways to treat disorders, improve learning, and explore human development. They didn’t have to guess what people thought—they could observe what they did.
So, defining psychology as a science of behavior gives it strength. It opens doors to research, application, and real-world solutions. While not the full story, behavior offers a solid foundation. And when paired with inner processes, it helps psychology stay scientific and useful.
5. Dr. Watson’s School of Psychology
Among all definitions of psychology, the most modern comes from behaviorism. This school of thought was shaped by Dr. John B. Watson. He brought a fresh view—one that challenged older ideas about mind and consciousness. Watson believed psychology should study only what can be seen: behavior.
He argued that thoughts and feelings are too vague. For him, states like decision-making or awareness weren’t scientifically reliable. If you couldn’t measure it, it didn’t belong in psychology. So, Watson pushed the field toward pure observation. He wanted psychology to act like other sciences—based on facts, not speculation.
According to him, consciousness was just a fancy word from the past. It was like “soul”—too rooted in tradition to be useful in modern research. Watson said, even if it exists, we can’t study it. We can’t test it or verify it. Therefore, psychology should avoid it altogether.
Behaviorism, especially in Watson’s form, focused on visible actions. It asked what people do, not what they feel deep inside. If someone flinched, cried, or smiled, that response became the data. This method was simple, direct, and practical. And it shaped American psychology for years.
However, Watson’s version was extreme. He ignored mental processes and emotions entirely. Critics felt this created an incomplete picture. After all, humans aren’t just machines—they think, dream, and reflect. So later psychologists tried to balance his ideas with deeper insight.
Still, Watson’s school changed psychology forever. It made the science more experimental. It gave it tools to work with. And it reminded researchers to stay grounded in observable fact. Even today, behaviorism influences how psychologists study learning, habits, and therapy.